306x Filetype PDF File size 1.06 MB Source: linguistics.ucla.edu
Introduction to Linguistics MarcusKracht Department of Linguistics, UCLA 3125CampbellHall 450Hilgard Avenue LosAngeles, CA 90095–1543 kracht@humnet.ucla.edu 2 Contents Contents Lecture 1: Introduction ................................................. 3 Lecture 2: Phonetics...................................................12 Lecture 3: Phonology I.................................................24 Lecture 4: Phonology II................................................41 Lecture 5: Phonology III...............................................55 Lecture 6: Phonology IV...............................................66 Lecture 7: Morphology I...............................................79 Lecture 8: Syntax I .................................................... 86 Lecture 9: Syntax II ................................................... 98 Lecture 10: Syntax III ................................................ 109 Lecture 11: Syntax IV ................................................ 119 Lecture 12: Syntax V ................................................. 134 Lecture 13: Morphology II............................................144 Lecture 14: Semantics I...............................................154 Lecture 15: Semantics II..............................................160 Lecture 16: Semantics III.............................................168 Lecture 17: Semantics IV ............................................. 177 Lecture 18: Semantics V..............................................186 Lecture 19: Language Families and History of Languages .............. 194 Lecture 1: Introduction Languages are sets of signs. Signs combine an exponent (a sequence of letters or sounds) with a meaning. Grammars are ways to generate signs from more basic signs. Signs combine a form and a meaning, andtheyareidenticalwithneithertheirexponentnorwiththeirmean- ing. Before we start. I have tried to be as explicit as I could in preparing these notes. You will find that some of the technicalities are demanding at first sight. Do not panic! You are not expected to master these technicalities right away. The technical character is basically due to my desire to be as explicit and detailed as possible. For some of you this might actually be helpful. If you are not among themyoumaywanttoreadsomeotherbookontheside(whichIencourageyouto doanyway). However,linguisticsisgettingincreasinglyformalandmathematical, and you are well advised to get used to this style of doing science. So, if you do not understand right away what I am saying, you will simply have to go over it again and again. And keep asking questions! New words and technical terms that are used for the first time are typed in bold-face. If you are supposed to know what they mean, a definition will be given right away. The definition is valid throughout the entire course, but be aware of the fact that other people might define things differently. This applies when you read other books, for example. Youshould beware of possible discrepancies in terminology. If you are not given a definition elsewhere, be cautious. If you are given a different definition it does not mean that the other books get it wrong. The symbol in the margin signals some material that is difficult, and optional. Such passages are put in for those whowanttogetaperfect understanding of the material; but they are not requried knowledge. (End of note) Language is a means to communicate, it is a semiotic system. By that we simply mean that it is a set of signs. Its A sign is a pair consisting—in the words of Ferdinand de Saussure—of a signifier and a signified. We prefer to call the signifier the exponent and the signified the meaning. For example, in English the string /dog/ is a signifier, and its signified is, say, doghood, or the set of all dogs. (I use the slashes to enclose concrete signifiers, in this case sequences of letters.) Sign systems are ubiquitous: clocks, road signs, pictograms—they all are parts of 4 Lecture 1: Introduction sign systems. Language differs from them only in its complexity. This explains why language signs have much more internal structure than ordinary signs. For notice that language allows to express virtually every thought that we have, and the number of signs that we can produce is literally endless. Although one may find it debatable whether or not language is actually infinite, it is clear that we are able to understand utterances that we have never heard before. Every year, hundreds of thousands of books appear, and clearly each of them is new. If it were the same as a previously published book this would be considered a breach of copyright! However, no native speaker of the language experiences trouble understanding them (apart from technical books). It might be far fetched, though, to speak of an entire book as a sign. But nothing speaks against that. Linguists mostly study only signs that consist of just one sentence. And this is what we shall do here, too. However, texts are certainly more than a sequence of sentences, and the study of discourse (which includes texts and dialogs) is certainly a very vital one. Unfortunately, even sentences are so complicated that it will take all our time to study them. The methods, however, shall be useful for discourse analysis as well. In linguistics, language signs are constituted of four different levels, not just two: phonology, morphology, syntax and semantics. Semantics deals with the meanings (what is signified), while the other three are all concerned with the exponent. At the lowest level we find that everything is composed from a small set of sounds, or—when we write—of letters. (Chinese is exceptional in that the alphabet consists of around 50,000 ‘letters’, but each sign stands for a syllable—a sequence of sounds, not just a single one.) With some exceptions (for example tone and intonation) every utterance can be seen as a sequence of sounds. For example, /dog/ consists of three letters (and three sounds): /d/, /o/ and /g/. In order not to confuse sounds (and sound sequences) with letters we denote the sounds by enclosing them in square brackets. So, the sounds that make up [dog] are [d], [o] and [g], in that order. What is important to note here is that sounds by themselves in general have no meaning. The decomposition into sounds has no counterpart in the semantics. Just as every signifier can be decomposed into sounds, it can also be decomposed into words. In written language we can spot the words by looking for minimal parts of texts enclosed by blanks (or punctuation marks). In spoken language the definition of word becomes very tricky. The part of linguistics that deals with how words are put together into sentences is called syntax. On the other hand, words are not the smallest meaningful units of
no reviews yet
Please Login to review.